Автор |
Kirsh, David |
Дата выпуска |
1996 |
dc.description |
This article examines some of the methods used by animals and humans to adapt their environment. Because there are limits on the number of different tasks a creature can be designed to do well in, creatures with the capacity to redesign their environments have an adaptive advantage over those who can adapt only passively to existing environmental structures. To clarify environmental redesign, l rely on the formal notion of a task environment as a directed graph in which the nodes are states and the links are actions. One natural form of redesign is to change the topology of this graph structure so as to increase the likelihood of task success or to reduce its expected cost, measured in physical terms. This may be done by eliminating initial states, hence eliminating choice points; by changing the action repertoire; by changing the consequence function; and, lastly, by adding choice points. Another major method for adapting the environment is to change its cognitive congeniality. Such changes leave the state space formally intact but reduce the number and cost of mental operations needed for task success; they reliably increase the speed, accuracy, or robustness of performance. The last section of the article describes several of these epistemic or complementary actions found in human performance. |
Издатель |
Sage Publications |
Тема |
adaptation |
Тема |
task environment |
Тема |
redesign |
Тема |
epistemic actions; complementary actions |
Название |
Adapting the Environment Instead of Oneself |
Тип |
Journal Article |
DOI |
10.1177/105971239600400307 |
Print ISSN |
1059-7123 |
Журнал |
Adaptive Behavior |
Том |
4 |
Первая страница |
415 |
Последняя страница |
452 |
Аффилиация |
Kirsh, David, Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California, University of California, San Diego |
Выпуск |
3-4 |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Batali, J., & Kitcher, P. (1995). Evolution of altruism in optional and compulsory games. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 175, 161-171. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Brandon, R.N. (1990). Adaptation and environment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Butler, D.R. (1995). Zoogeomorplcology: Animals as geomorphic agents . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Diaper, D. (Ed.). (1989). Task analysis for human computer interaction. Chichester, UK: Ellis Horwood. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Duffy, T. M., & Jonassen, D. H. (Eds.). (1992). Constructivism and the technology of instruction: A conversation. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Elster, J. (1979). Ulysses and the sirens: Studies in rationality and irrationality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Fisher, Sir R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Gigerenzer, G., & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve Bayesian reasoning without instruction: Frequency formats. Psychological Review, 102, 684-704. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Hammond, K.J., Converse, T.M., & Grass, J.W. (1995). The stabilization of environments. Artificial Intelligence, 72, 305-327. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Harel, D. (1987). Algorithmics: The spirit of computation. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Horn, H.S. (1979). Adaptation from the perspective of optimality . In A. T. Solbrig, S. Jain, G. B. Johnson & P. H. Raven (Eds.), Topics in plant population biology. New York: Columbia University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Howard, R.A. (1966). Information value theory. IEEE Transactions on Systems Science and Cybernetics, 2, 22-26. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Hunt, G.R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows [letter to editor]. Nature, 379, 249. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kirsh, D. (1990). When is information explicitly represented? In P Hanson (Ed.), Information, language, and cognition: The Vancouver studies in cognitive science (vol 1). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kirsh, D. (1995a). Complementary strategies: Why we use our hands when we think. In Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kirsh, D. (1995b). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73, 31-68. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic actions . Cognitive Science, 18, 513-549. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Koza, J.R., Rice, J.P., & Roughgarden, J. (1992). Evolution of food-foraging strategies for the Caribbean Anolis lizard using genetic programming. Adaptive Beltavior, 1(2), 171-199. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kroemer, K.H.E. (1993). Fitting the workplace to the human and not vice versa (use of ergonomics in work area design). Industrial Engineering, 25(3), 56. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Kummar, H. (1995). In quest of the sacred baboon: A scientist's journey (M. A. Biederman-Thorson, Trans.) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Lao-Tze. (1898). Tao-teh-king, Chinese- English. (With introduction and transliteration and notes by Paul Carus.) Chicago: Open Court. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Larkin, J.H., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth ten thousand words. Cognitive Science, 11(1), 65-100. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Lewontin, R.C. (1978). Adaptation and natural selection. Scientific American, September. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
McFarland, D., & Houston, A. (1981). Quantitative ethology: The state space approach . Boston: Pitman Advanced Publishing Program. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Newell, A. (1973). Production systems: Model of control structure . In W. C. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Newell, A., & Simon, H.A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Norman, D.A. (1988). The psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Norman, D.A. (1991). Cognitive artifacts. In J. M. Carroll (Ed.), Designing interaction: Psychology at the human-computer interface. [Cambridge series on human-computer interaction, no. 4.] Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Papadimitriou, C. (1995). Computational complexity. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Royama, T. (1992). Analytical population dynamics. London: Chapman & Hall. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H. (1955). Behavior and the structure of the environment . [Reprinted in H. Simon, Models of thought (vol 1), 1984.] |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment . Psychological Review, 63:129-38. [Reprinted in H. Simon, Models of thought (vol 1), 1984.] |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H. (1973). The structure of ill-structured problems. Artificial Intelligence, 4, 181-201. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H. (1981a). The sciences of the artificial (2nd ed). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H. (1981b). The architecture of complexity. [Reprinted in The sciences of the artificial (2nd ed).] Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Simon, H.A. (1976). Administrative behavior: A study of decision-making processes in administrative organization (3rd ed). New York: Free Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Suchinan, L. (1986). Situated cognition. Cambridge , UK: Cambridge University Press. |
Библиографическая ссылка |
Teichner, W.H., & Olson, D.E. (1971). A preliminary theory of the effects of task and environmental factors on human performance. Human Factors , 13(4), 295-344. |